Tuesday, April 16th, 2024
 
 
 
Updates automatically
Twitter Link
CHN iOS App
 
NCAA
1967 1970

ECAC
1967 1968 1969 1970 1973 1980 1986 1996 1997 2003 2005 2010

IVY
1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1977 1978 1983 1984 1985 1996 1997 2002 2003 2004 2005 2012 2014

Cleary Jell-O Mold
2002 2003 2005

Ned Harkness Cup
2003 2005 2008 2013
 
Brendon
Iles
Pokulok
Schafer
Syphilis

2018 ECAC Permutations

Posted by Give My Regards 
Page:  1 2Next
Current Page: 1 of 2
2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Give My Regards (---.twcny.res.rr.com)
Date: February 18, 2018 11:38PM

Here we go again, folks. First the short version:

Possible ECAC tournament seedings (the number in parentheses is the best
seed the team can get with no help):

Cornell        1-2    (1)
Union          1-3    (2)
Clarkson       2-4    (3)
Harvard        3-4    (4)
Dartmouth      5-9    (5)
Colgate        5-9    (6)
Yale           5-9    (7)
Princeton      5-9    (7)
Quinnipiac     5-9    (9)
Brown         10-11   (10)
Rensselaer    10-12   (11)
St. Lawrence  11-12   (12)

 
___________________________
If you lead a good life, go to Sunday school and church, and say your prayers every night, when you die, you'll go to LYNAH!
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Give My Regards (---.twcny.res.rr.com)
Date: February 18, 2018 11:41PM

...and now the grotesquely long version:

Once again, it's time for the ECAC Playoff Permutations!  Oddly enough,
this year the teams have slotted themselves nicely into three tiers; there
are four teams that can finish in first through fourth, five that can
finish from fifth through ninth, and three that can finish from tenth
through twelfth.  If you love drama, the final ECAC weekend is a little
short of if this year, although it makes things a lot easier to figure out.

Going into the final weekend of league play, here's a breakdown of where
each team in the ECAC could finish.  As always, I'm greatly indebted to
John Whelan's excellent playoff possibilities script at [www.elynah.com]


For each ECAC team, I've listed the following:

THIS WEEKEND:  The team's weekend games, its last two of the season.
ON THEIR OWN:  The highest the team could finish with no help from the
      competition.  Generally, this involves a weekend sweep.
BEST CASE:  The highest the team could finish if everything goes right.
WORST CASE:  The lowest the team could finish if everything goes wrong.
      This generally involves getting swept while teams nearby in the
      standings win.
TIEBREAKERS:  How the team would fare if they finished the season tied with
      some other team which is currently close (i.e. within 4 points) in the
      standings.  Note that there may be cases in which Team A "could win or
      lose" the tiebreaker against Team B, depending on whether there are
      more than just those two teams tied.  For instance, Colgate wins the
      head-to-head tiebreaker against Princeton with a 1-0-1 record; however,
      in a four-way tie involving these two, Yale, and Dartmouth, Colgate
      would actually be seeded lower than Princeton.  If a listed tiebreaker
      result depends on more than just those two teams being tied, it is
      marked with an asterisk:

           Colgate could win or lose* against Princeton

For two or more teams tied in the standings, the ECAC tiebreakers are:

1.   Head-to-head record in ECAC games (non-conference meetings, such as in
     tournaments, do not count).
2.   League wins.
3.   Record against the top four teams in the conference.
4.   Record against the top eight teams in the conference.
5.   Goal differential (net goals) head-to-head.
6.   Goal differential against the top four teams in the conference.
7.   Goal differential against the top eight teams in the conference.

Note that if the tie is among three or more teams, the tiebreaking steps are
used in order until a team, or multiple teams, is/are separated from the
"pack".  Once that happens, the process starts all over to break the remaining
ties.  For example, when the above steps are applied to a four-way tie, once
one team is separated out leaving a three-way tie, the procedure goes back to
the first step with the three remaining tied teams.


Without further ado, here's how the final weekend looks:

Cornell:
      THIS WEEKEND:  At Rensselaer, at Union.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Wraps up first place with one point on the weekend.
      BEST CASE:  First.
      WORST CASE:  Will finish second with two losses if Union also beats
      Colgate.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Loses to Union.

Union:
      THIS WEEKEND:  Colgate, Cornell.
      ON THEIR OWN:  One point will clinch second place.
      BEST CASE:  Finishes first with a sweep if Cornell also loses to
      Rensselaer.
      WORST CASE:  Drops to third if they lose twice and Clarkson sweeps.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats Cornell; loses to Clarkson.

Clarkson:
      THIS WEEKEND:  Princeton, Quinnipiac.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Clinches third with a pair of wins.
      BEST CASE:  Climbs to second with two wins if Union loses twice.
      WORST CASE:  Falls to fourth with two losses if Harvard gets at least
      one point.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats Union; loses to Harvard.

Harvard:
      THIS WEEKEND:  At Brown, at Yale.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Has already wrapped up fourth place and can do no 
      better without help.
      BEST CASE:  Takes third with a sweep if Clarkson gets no more than three
      points.
      WORST CASE:  Fourth.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats Clarkson and Dartmouth.

Dartmouth:
      THIS WEEKEND:  At Yale, at Brown.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Will guarantee fifth place with a sweep.
      BEST CASE:  Fifth.
      WORST CASE:  Would slide to ninth with two losses if Quinnipiac wins
      twice and Colgate and Princeton each get at least two points.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats Quinnipiac; loses to Princeton; could win or lose
      against Colgate and Yale.

Colgate:
      THIS WEEKEND:  At Union, at Rensselaer.
      ON THEIR OWN:  A sweep wraps up sixth place.
      BEST CASE:  Rises to fifth with two wins if Dartmouth does not sweep.
      WORST CASE:  Would finish ninth with two losses if Yale gets at least
      one point, Princeton gets at least two points, and Quinnipiac gets at
      least three points.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Loses to Yale; could win or lose against Dartmouth and
      Quinnipiac; could win or lose* against Princeton.

Yale:
      THIS WEEKEND:  Dartmouth, Harvard.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Clinches seventh with a pair of wins.
      BEST CASE:  Climbs to fifth if they win twice, Dartmouth does not beat
      Brown, and Colgate does not sweep.
      WORST CASE:  Falls to ninth if they lose twice, Princeton does not get
      swept, and Quinnipiac gets at least two points.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats Colgate and Quinnipiac; could win or lose against
      Dartmouth and Princeton.

Princeton:
      THIS WEEKEND:  At Clarkson, at St. Lawrence.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Guarantees seventh with two wins.
      BEST CASE:  Would finish fifth with a sweep if Colgate gets no more
      than two points and the Dartmouth-Yale winner loses its other game
      (or if they tie, Dartmouth does not win its other game).
      WORST CASE:  Slides to ninth if they lose twice and Quinnipiac gets
      at least two points.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats Dartmouth; could win or lose against Yale and
      Quinnipiac; could win* or lose against Colgate.

Quinnipiac:
      THIS WEEKEND:  At St. Lawrence, at Clarkson.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Has clinched ninth and can do no better without help.
      BEST CASE:  Takes fifth with a sweep if Dartmouth loses twice, Colgate
      gets no more than one point, Princeton gets no more than two points,
      and Yale loses to Harvard.
      WORST CASE:  Ninth.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Loses to Dartmouth and Yale; could win or lose against
      Colgate and Princeton.

Brown:
      THIS WEEKEND:  Harvard, Dartmouth.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Guarantees tenth with a three-point weekend.
      BEST CASE:  Tenth.
      WORST CASE:  Drops to eleventh if they lose twice and Rensselaer
      gets at least two points.
      TIEBREAKERS:   Beats Rensselaer and St. Lawrence.

Rensselaer:
      THIS WEEKEND:  Cornell, Colgate.
      ON THEIR OWN:  One point would give the Engineers eleventh place.
      BEST CASE:  Gets tenth with a sweep if Brown gets no more than two
      points.
      WORST CASE:  Would finish twelfth if they lose twice and St. Lawrence
      wins twice.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Beats St. Lawrence; loses to Brown.

St. Lawrence:
      THIS WEEKEND:  Quinnipiac, Princeton.
      ON THEIR OWN:  Can do no better than twelfth without help.
      BEST CASE:  Finishes eleventh with a sweep if Rensselaer loses twice.
      WORST CASE:  Twelfth.
      TIEBREAKERS:  Loses to Brown and Rensselaer.

 
___________________________
If you lead a good life, go to Sunday school and church, and say your prayers every night, when you die, you'll go to LYNAH!
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:20AM

Give My Regards
...and now the grotesquely long version:

Wow, impressed and thankful, but how long did it take you?

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: scoop85 (---.hvc.res.rr.com)
Date: February 19, 2018 10:22AM

Not strictly ECAC related, but this seems to confirm we’re in the NCAA’s regardless of what happens the rest of the way:

 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: KenP (107.77.76.---)
Date: February 19, 2018 10:44AM

scoop85
Not strictly ECAC related, but this seems to confirm we’re in the NCAA’s regardless of what happens the rest of the way:

. Also shows 91% likelihood of being a #1 seed.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: upprdeck (---.fs.cornell.edu)
Date: February 19, 2018 01:20PM

but this doesn show what happens if we do lose the next 4. just the likelihood of that are low.. even 2-4 probably keeps us high.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: imafrshmn (---.earthsat.com)
Date: February 19, 2018 02:30PM

upprdeck
but this doesn show what happens if we do lose the next 4. just the likelihood of that are low.. even 2-4 probably keeps us high.

This is true. As well, there's reason to believe that the CHN simulations don't really capture the low-probability outcomes as well as they should (which is to say it's all a little bit overconfident). Because KRACH ratings are assumed to be (1) constant, (2) unbiased, and (3) not-uncertain, there is no way to account for recent trends (like Clarkson tanking, for example), variations in luck, and inherent uncertainty/variability of a team's strength. Of course, these assumptions simplify this exercise to a point where it's easily understood.

 
___________________________
class of '09
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 02:38PM

imafrshmn
... there's reason to believe that the CHN simulations don't really capture the low-probability outcomes as well as they should (which is to say it's all a little bit overconfident). Because KRACH ratings are assumed to be (1) constant, (2) unbiased, and (3) not-uncertain, there is no way to account for recent trends (like Clarkson tanking, for example), variations in luck, and inherent uncertainty/variability of a team's strength. Of course, these assumptions simplify this exercise to a point where it's easily understood.

repeating: anyone wants to help write a better algorithm, be my guest.

All of the above is true. 1. can be solved easily, but requires a lot of additional computing time - way too much to be worth it. 3. there's a lot packed there. Some would be good to adjust for, others have more dubious value
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: upprdeck (---.fs.cornell.edu)
Date: February 19, 2018 03:35PM

wouldnt it require the ability to run the 20K simulations with the 1 know result?
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 03:48PM

upprdeck
wouldnt it require the ability to run the 20K simulations with the 1 know result?

You could do it where the KRACH gets re-computed after each 'day' in the simulation ... but that would require like 20 KRACH computations per simulation - multiplied by 20,000
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: upprdeck (---.fs.cornell.edu)
Date: February 19, 2018 04:14PM

you mean like the power of mining for crypto currancy
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jeff Hopkins '82 (---.37.26.69.virtela.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 05:16PM

if we have to finish #2, I'd like it to be behind St. Cloud or Mankato. Extra likelihood of us being in Allentown.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 06:27PM

adamw
imafrshmn
... there's reason to believe that the CHN simulations don't really capture the low-probability outcomes as well as they should (which is to say it's all a little bit overconfident). Because KRACH ratings are assumed to be (1) constant, (2) unbiased, and (3) not-uncertain, there is no way to account for recent trends (like Clarkson tanking, for example), variations in luck, and inherent uncertainty/variability of a team's strength. Of course, these assumptions simplify this exercise to a point where it's easily understood.

repeating: anyone wants to help write a better algorithm, be my guest.

All of the above is true. 1. can be solved easily, but requires a lot of additional computing time - way too much to be worth it. 3. there's a lot packed there. Some would be good to adjust for, others have more dubious value
adamw, I wish I had the mathematical or computing background to help improve the algorithm. I can only say that the 59% chance the model gives us of winning the ECAC tournament has to be wrong. 40% would already be pushing it. Someone want to go back and check how often a team whom this model gave a "60% chance" of winning an 8-team tournament didn't end up winning it?
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/19/2018 06:30PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: abmarks (---.hsd1.ma.comcast.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:03PM

BearLover
adamw
imafrshmn
... there's reason to believe that the CHN simulations don't really capture the low-probability outcomes as well as they should (which is to say it's all a little bit overconfident). Because KRACH ratings are assumed to be (1) constant, (2) unbiased, and (3) not-uncertain, there is no way to account for recent trends (like Clarkson tanking, for example), variations in luck, and inherent uncertainty/variability of a team's strength. Of course, these assumptions simplify this exercise to a point where it's easily understood.

repeating: anyone wants to help write a better algorithm, be my guest.

All of the above is true. 1. can be solved easily, but requires a lot of additional computing time - way too much to be worth it. 3. there's a lot packed there. Some would be good to adjust for, others have more dubious value
adamw, I wish I had the mathematical or computing background to help improve the algorithm. I can only say that the 59% chance the model gives us of winning the ECAC tournament has to be wrong. 40% would already be pushing it. Someone want to go back and check how often a team whom this model gave a "60% chance" of winning an 8-team tournament didn't end up winning it?

The frigging model is 100% correct, BearLover. TLDR version: Bearlover doesn't understand instructions.

Bearlover, If you'd read the explanation of the model, it clearly states that:

CHN says:
These are the results of 20,000 Monte Carlo simulations of the remaining games prior to Selection Day. The winner of each game in the simulation was determined randomly, weighted by KRACH.

The simple translation of this is that, based on the relative value of the KRACH ratings, IN THE LONG RUN (when variance is removed) We are expected to win 59% of the time. I.e. we'd be expected to win 11,800 of the 20,000 times. So, as defined, the model is accurate.

This model can't possibly be used to tell you the likelihood of wining the tournament when the tournament is only played out once, not 20,000 times. Because, variance.

Please stop shitting on the model as wrong. It's not. It is what it is.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Dafatone (---.sub-174-217-20.myvzw.com)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:14PM

I think it's easy to believe that we are worse than our record, given that we've slowed down a little bit as of late, we're beaten up, and that all year, we seem to win a lot of close games.

But KRACH goes by record, not recent record or margin of victory. And our record is really, really, REALLY good.

I'd expect the 2nd best team in the country to win its tournament often, especially given that the next best teams are 8th and 21st. And KRACH says we're #2.

Are we actually #2? I have no idea. I haven't been watching other teams all that often. I have to figure that the extreme difference between conferences is at least a little bit flukey, and that the ECAC isn't as far behind the others as the results have said. But that really works in our favor, not against.

We've given up 38 goals in 27 games. That's absurd. Maybe we aren't as good as our performance, but our performance has been fantastic.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Trotsky (---.washdc.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:40PM

It is basic human psychology to look at a mathematical model that produces a counter-intuitive event probability and think "that model must be wrong."

It could be wrong, of course, but the "mustness" of the feeling inverts reality. When a well-developed algorithm conflicts with your assessment of likelihood it's most likely indicating that your brain is wrong. There are a host of cognitive biases that make our seat-of-the-pants judgments prone to error.
Edited 2 time(s). Last edit at 02/19/2018 07:44PM by Trotsky.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:40PM

abmarks
BearLover
adamw
imafrshmn
... there's reason to believe that the CHN simulations don't really capture the low-probability outcomes as well as they should (which is to say it's all a little bit overconfident). Because KRACH ratings are assumed to be (1) constant, (2) unbiased, and (3) not-uncertain, there is no way to account for recent trends (like Clarkson tanking, for example), variations in luck, and inherent uncertainty/variability of a team's strength. Of course, these assumptions simplify this exercise to a point where it's easily understood.

repeating: anyone wants to help write a better algorithm, be my guest.

All of the above is true. 1. can be solved easily, but requires a lot of additional computing time - way too much to be worth it. 3. there's a lot packed there. Some would be good to adjust for, others have more dubious value
adamw, I wish I had the mathematical or computing background to help improve the algorithm. I can only say that the 59% chance the model gives us of winning the ECAC tournament has to be wrong. 40% would already be pushing it. Someone want to go back and check how often a team whom this model gave a "60% chance" of winning an 8-team tournament didn't end up winning it?

The frigging model is 100% correct, BearLover. TLDR version: Bearlover doesn't understand instructions.

Bearlover, If you'd read the explanation of the model, it clearly states that:

CHN says:
These are the results of 20,000 Monte Carlo simulations of the remaining games prior to Selection Day. The winner of each game in the simulation was determined randomly, weighted by KRACH.

The simple translation of this is that, based on the relative value of the KRACH ratings, IN THE LONG RUN (when variance is removed) We are expected to win 59% of the time. I.e. we'd be expected to win 11,800 of the 20,000 times. So, as defined, the model is accurate.

This model can't possibly be used to tell you the likelihood of wining the tournament when the tournament is only played out once, not 20,000 times. Because, variance.

Please stop shitting on the model as wrong. It's not. It is what it is.
Hey buddy, I understand what variance is and that no model can predict what will happen in any single instance. I am saying that over 20,000 ECAC tournaments, we would win it fewer than 11,8000 times. Sorry your attempt at being condescending missed the entire premise of my post.

You say, "as defined, the model is accurate." That's some impressively circular logic! If this predictor is, as you say, "based on the relative value of the KRACH ratings," my issue is with KRACH as a predictor of hockey games, not with the model's application of KRACH (I thought that was obvious from my initial post, but guess not). Specifically, my issue is that KRACH fails to account for the variance over a small sample size of games leading up to this point in the season. There is a very low degree of certainty that Cornell is actually the second-best team in the country. We could realistically be anywhere from 1-25. Does KRACH--and, by extension, the model--account for that?
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/19/2018 07:41PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:42PM

Trotsky
It is basic human psychology to look at a mathematical model that produces a counter-intuitive event probability and think "that model must be wrong."

It could be wrong, of course, but the "mustness" of the feeling inverts reality. When a well-developed algorithm conflicts with your assessment of likelihood it's indicating that your brain is wrong. There are a host of cognitive biases that make our seat-of-the-pants judgments prone to error.
Yeah, we know, we're past this point.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Trotsky (---.washdc.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:46PM

BearLover
Trotsky
It is basic human psychology to look at a mathematical model that produces a counter-intuitive event probability and think "that model must be wrong."

It could be wrong, of course, but the "mustness" of the feeling inverts reality. When a well-developed algorithm conflicts with your assessment of likelihood it's indicating that your brain is wrong. There are a host of cognitive biases that make our seat-of-the-pants judgments prone to error.
Yeah, we know, we're past this point.
Evidently not.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 07:58PM

Trotsky
BearLover
Trotsky
It is basic human psychology to look at a mathematical model that produces a counter-intuitive event probability and think "that model must be wrong."

It could be wrong, of course, but the "mustness" of the feeling inverts reality. When a well-developed algorithm conflicts with your assessment of likelihood it's indicating that your brain is wrong. There are a host of cognitive biases that make our seat-of-the-pants judgments prone to error.
Yeah, we know, we're past this point.
Evidently not.
The only person (erroneously) arguing this point is abmarks, who misinterpreted my argument. This model doesn't just look wrong--it looks heinously wrong. We can't know for sure until someone checks it against empirical data (past tournaments, as I suggested above). AFAIK, KRACH isn't meant to be predictive. In fact, from what I understand, it fails very badly at being predictive. No one is saying "this model is wrong because in my experience [this number] cannot be true!" People are saying, "this model looks very wrong, [this improperly weighted input] is probably why, someone please check it against empirical data to be sure."
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: jfeath17 (---.dyn.optonline.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 09:32PM

I started doing some analysis on how accurate using KRACH is to predict results. I will follow up in the next post with some more details on what I did, but the basic gist was I collected the KRACH based projected winning percentage for the better ranked team and the result of the game (win/tie/loss). I then used a logistic regression to this data using the KRACH prediction as the independent variable. I used the results of 1129 games over the two previous seasons.

KRACH | Result
------+-------
0.50 | 0.5390
0.55 | 0.5750
0.60 | 0.6102
0.65 | 0.6444
0.70 | 0.6771
0.75 | 0.7081
0.80 | 0.7374
0.85 | 0.7647
0.90 | 0.7899
0.95 | 0.8131
1.00 | 0.8343

 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: jfeath17 (---.dyn.optonline.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 10:04PM

Some more details:

I used data from the past two complete seasons. I hope to add more seasons, but the dates for games on USCHO from 3 years ago seem to be in a mix of d/m/y and m/d/y which kinda breaks things. (On that note, if anyone knows of a easily parsable database of game results that would be great since I'm currently copying the table from USCHO into excel and exporting it as a csv.)

I step through the schedule week by week and update the KRACH rating for every team. Then , I calculate the KRACH based projection of winning percentage for the upcoming week's games. I always use the higher ranked teams winning likelihood so they are all within the 0.5-1 range. I then save the result of the game (tie, higher ranked won, higher ranked loss) along with the KRACH based likelihood. I start this process at the beginning of January to ignore the early season variability of KRACH (also to avoid any of the complexities of calculating KRACH on undefeated teams). Now that I am typing this up I realize that stepping through on a weekly basis isn't really necessary and I am thinking about changing it to a day by day step.

Now I am left with two variables, KRACH Prediction Win Likelihood and the actual result of the game. I tried a couple different things here to try to find a good correlation between the two. Based on my research, I think the best way is to use a Logistic Regression and those are the results shown in the above post. I don't consider myself an expert in this stuff at all so I very well could be making some bad assumptions here. If anyone has a better method to compare them, I'm interested to hear.

If anyone has any questions or suggestions for further things to try out, I'd love to hear them.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: abmarks (---.hsd1.ma.comcast.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 10:58PM

BearLover
Trotsky
BearLover
Trotsky
It is basic human psychology to look at a mathematical model that produces a counter-intuitive event probability and think "that model must be wrong."

It could be wrong, of course, but the "mustness" of the feeling inverts reality. When a well-developed algorithm conflicts with your assessment of likelihood it's indicating that your brain is wrong. There are a host of cognitive biases that make our seat-of-the-pants judgments prone to error.
Yeah, we know, we're past this point.
Evidently not.
The only person (erroneously) arguing this point is abmarks, who misinterpreted my argument. This model doesn't just look wrong--it looks heinously wrong. We can't know for sure until someone checks it against empirical data (past tournaments, as I suggested above). AFAIK, KRACH isn't meant to be predictive. In fact, from what I understand, it fails very badly at being predictive. No one is saying "this model is wrong because in my experience [this number] cannot be true!" People are saying, "this model looks very wrong, [this improperly weighted input] is probably why, someone please check it against empirical data to be sure."

There will never be enough tourney results to create a dataset big enough to generate an empirical conclusion of any precision, once again, because variance.


If you weren't so lazy, you might have read the FAQ page at CHN, which was answered by JTW (of this forum) himself. [www.collegehockeynews.com] . Here is part of it, with key info bolded and italicised.


Q. Can you tell us a little more?

A: Getting a bit more technical: The Bradley-Terry system is based on a statistical technique called logistic regression, in essence meaning that teams' ratings are determined directly from their won-loss records against one another. KRACH's strength of schedule component is calculated directly from the ratings themselves, which is a key point. It means that KRACH, unlike many ratings (including RPI), cannot easily be distorted by teams with strong records against weak opposition.

The ratings are on an odds scale, so if Team A's KRACH rating is three times as large as Team B's, Team A would be expected to amass a winning percentage of .750 and Team B a winning percentage of .250 if it played each other enough times. The correct ratings are defined such that the "expected" winning percentage for a team in the games it's already played is equal to its "actual" winning percentage.

Q. And so why is this so great?

A: In other words, if you took one team's schedule to date, and played a theoretical "game" for each game already actually played, using the KRACH ratings themselves in order to predict the winner, then the end result would be a theoretical won-loss percentage that matches the team's actual won-loss percentage. Pretty cool.

It is not possible to do any better than that with a completely objective method. Any other method would introduce arbitrary-ness and/or subjectivity.

Q. What are the limitations?

A: Well, KRACH can't predict the future. Nothing can. The idea behind such ratings systems is to use them in order to properly select and seed tournaments. Champions are then determined on the ice. All systems are designed to analyze past results, not necessarily predict future ones. Though, by theory, the more sound the analysis of the past, the better the ability to predict future results.

KRACH is "perfect" in its analysis of past results. But that should not be construed to mean that it definitively decides which team is better. When dealing with sample sizes like this, you never know. Team A could lose to Team B, be below them in KRACH, and then turn around and beat Team B the next three times. KRACH would then change. It does not invalidate what KRACH represented at the time, however.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 19, 2018 11:34PM

abmarks
If you weren't so lazy, you might have read the FAQ page at CHN, which was answered by JTW (of this forum) himself. [www.collegehockeynews.com] . Here is part of it, with key info bolded and italicised.
You'll be happy to know I read that entire primer before my initial posts. It doesn't answer any of my questions or help your case in any way.

abmarks
There will never be enough tourney results to create a dataset big enough to generate an empirical conclusion of any precision, once again, because variance.
There exists data from hundreds of tournaments and thousands of games that we can compare against KRACH-based predictions.

You're also being obtuse in you emboldening/italicizing of clauses from the KRACH FAQ (thanks, by the way!). Yeah, KRACH isn't meant to be predictive. And yeah, nothing can better objectively measure past results. No one cares about those things. The question at hand is whether KRACH happens to be predictive to a significant enough degree that it's worth using in models that predict outcomes of hockey games. That was always the question, not whether KRACH is a nice way of seeding for tournaments or whether this predictor misapplied KRACH. Since there is absolutely nothing, here in this thread, or included in the KRACH FAQ, or gleaned from comparing this model against other sports/hockey prediction models, to suggest that KRACH is even a remotely good predictor of future hockey game outcomes, I'm going to assume KRACH is not a good predictor of future hockey game outcomes, and that therefore this model isn't good. Happy to be proven otherwise (in hopefully a more polite manner).

Why are you in such a foul mood, anyway?
Edited 4 time(s). Last edit at 02/19/2018 11:50PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: David Harding (---.hsd1.il.comcast.net)
Date: February 20, 2018 12:45AM

jfeath17
Some more details:

I used data from the past two complete seasons. I hope to add more seasons, but the dates for games on USCHO from 3 years ago seem to be in a mix of d/m/y and m/d/y which kinda breaks things. (On that note, if anyone knows of a easily parsable database of game results that would be great since I'm currently copying the table from USCHO into excel and exporting it as a csv.)

I step through the schedule week by week and update the KRACH rating for every team. Then , I calculate the KRACH based projection of winning percentage for the upcoming week's games. I always use the higher ranked teams winning likelihood so they are all within the 0.5-1 range. I then save the result of the game (tie, higher ranked won, higher ranked loss) along with the KRACH based likelihood. I start this process at the beginning of January to ignore the early season variability of KRACH (also to avoid any of the complexities of calculating KRACH on undefeated teams). Now that I am typing this up I realize that stepping through on a weekly basis isn't really necessary and I am thinking about changing it to a day by day step.

Now I am left with two variables, KRACH Prediction Win Likelihood and the actual result of the game. I tried a couple different things here to try to find a good correlation between the two. Based on my research, I think the best way is to use a Logistic Regression and those are the results shown in the above post. I don't consider myself an expert in this stuff at all so I very well could be making some bad assumptions here. If anyone has a better method to compare them, I'm interested to hear.

If anyone has any questions or suggestions for further things to try out, I'd love to hear them.

Thank you for tackling the question with data. This may be too simplistic, but how about sorting the games into 10 bins based on the predicted probability of the the favored team winning. 0.50<=x<0.55, 0.55<=x<0.60, etc. For each bin, calculate the fraction of the games that the favored team won. Graph the actual fraction vs the calculated fraction. You could multiply the number of games in each bin by the predicted fraction for the favored team to get the predicted number, then take the square root to get some notion of the size of the predicted error.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Beeeej (Moderator)
Date: February 20, 2018 08:16AM

Isn't it wonderful that we're arguing so vehemently over whether the statistical models are accurate when they say we're fantastic because our record is 22-3-2?

I definitely don't recall having these arguments in 1993. "No, we must be worse than our 6-19-1 record would suggest!!"

 
___________________________
Beeeej, Esq.

"Cornell isn't an organization. It's a loose affiliation of independent fiefdoms united by a common hockey team."
- Steve Worona
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: KenP (137.75.68.---)
Date: February 20, 2018 08:38AM

Is there a way to factor in the odds of a tie? I know it is incorporated into B-T ratings... but my guess is that hockey's non-binary results are a significant source of forecast error.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Swampy (---.ri.ri.cox.net)
Date: February 20, 2018 10:20AM

David Harding
jfeath17
Some more details:

I used data from the past two complete seasons. I hope to add more seasons, but the dates for games on USCHO from 3 years ago seem to be in a mix of d/m/y and m/d/y which kinda breaks things. (On that note, if anyone knows of a easily parsable database of game results that would be great since I'm currently copying the table from USCHO into excel and exporting it as a csv.)

I step through the schedule week by week and update the KRACH rating for every team. Then , I calculate the KRACH based projection of winning percentage for the upcoming week's games. I always use the higher ranked teams winning likelihood so they are all within the 0.5-1 range. I then save the result of the game (tie, higher ranked won, higher ranked loss) along with the KRACH based likelihood. I start this process at the beginning of January to ignore the early season variability of KRACH (also to avoid any of the complexities of calculating KRACH on undefeated teams). Now that I am typing this up I realize that stepping through on a weekly basis isn't really necessary and I am thinking about changing it to a day by day step.

Now I am left with two variables, KRACH Prediction Win Likelihood and the actual result of the game. I tried a couple different things here to try to find a good correlation between the two. Based on my research, I think the best way is to use a Logistic Regression and those are the results shown in the above post. I don't consider myself an expert in this stuff at all so I very well could be making some bad assumptions here. If anyone has a better method to compare them, I'm interested to hear.

If anyone has any questions or suggestions for further things to try out, I'd love to hear them.

Thank you for tackling the question with data. This may be too simplistic, but how about sorting the games into 10 bins based on the predicted probability of the the favored team winning. 0.50<=x<0.55, 0.55<=x<0.60, etc. For each bin, calculate the fraction of the games that the favored team won. Graph the actual fraction vs the calculated fraction. You could multiply the number of games in each bin by the predicted fraction for the favored team to get the predicted number, then take the square root to get some notion of the size of the predicted error.

Some things I'd want to add to the discussion:

1. We predict the future all the time, usually with good results. Do you want to bet the sun won't come up tomorrow? That if I take an umbrella in the rain I'll get less wet than without one? Etc.

2. Exactly how does variance play out in these methods. If Team A plays Team B, does the P[Team A or Team B wins] = 1.0? Suppose Team A has P[winning] = 0.6, and Team B has 0.4, but Team A is erratic (I'm looking at you Clarkson), while Team B is not. Does A's greater variance show up in the prediction?

3. Looking at Clarkson again, these things are time series. So recent performance should be weighted more heavily. Is it?

4. What about using other information besides past performance? Donato away at the Olympics? Cornell's top D-men and third-leading scorer are injured? Surely a pro from Las Vegas would use such information to handicap a team.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Tom Lento (199.201.64.---)
Date: February 20, 2018 02:36PM

BearLover
Trotsky
BearLover
Trotsky
It is basic human psychology to look at a mathematical model that produces a counter-intuitive event probability and think "that model must be wrong."

It could be wrong, of course, but the "mustness" of the feeling inverts reality. When a well-developed algorithm conflicts with your assessment of likelihood it's indicating that your brain is wrong. There are a host of cognitive biases that make our seat-of-the-pants judgments prone to error.
Yeah, we know, we're past this point.
Evidently not.
The only person (erroneously) arguing this point is abmarks, who misinterpreted my argument. This model doesn't just look wrong--it looks heinously wrong. We can't know for sure until someone checks it against empirical data (past tournaments, as I suggested above). AFAIK, KRACH isn't meant to be predictive. In fact, from what I understand, it fails very badly at being predictive. No one is saying "this model is wrong because in my experience [this number] cannot be true!" People are saying, "this model looks very wrong, [this improperly weighted input] is probably why, someone please check it against empirical data to be sure."

BearLover's complaints about the predictive model are valid, at least in terms of modeling decisions. The model in CHN (and playoffstatus, which is not meaningfully different) is based on a number of fairly strong assumptions, some of which are hidden, and that bears examining.

If it's meant to be an exercise in generating a valid distribution of outcomes without regard to predictive accuracy of empirical results, perhaps with the aim of starting conversation and giving fans something to gas about on weekdays, there's absolutely nothing wrong with it. If it's meant to be an effective predictor of tournament qualification and end of season results, it is questionable. I don't have the empirical data to know if it's *right* and I don't feel like doing that analysis, but the odds don't pass the sniff test. At this point, the predictions are so confident at the top end of the distribution that someone really would need to produce empirical outcomes showing the model's effectiveness before I'd believe those numbers. This doesn't mean I don't understand probability. It means I do understand modeling choices.

Failing to update KRACH along the way is a modeling choice everybody has talked about - that's actually a bad one because you're basically starting with a strong prior and failing to update it in any way. I understand the computational cost issues in play here but there's got to be a way to do that efficiently. If not, you could use something that has a lot of KRACH's desirable properties without the computational complexity (maybe Elo is better for this?) and then see how the models compare. That'll at least get you some sense of how much impact this decision has on your distributions.

Personally, I suspect the bigger problem with the predictive model CHN is using is it treats KRACH as 100% accurate - all of the variance between KRACH's predictions and actual empirical results are missing from the model itself. Excluding that input makes the model over-confident with respect to empirical predictors, and I think that's something you could address. At the very least you can externally validate the underlying assumption. There's plenty of data at this point - you can actually just do single point KRACH predictions and compare that with the distribution of empirical outcomes. There's no need to restrict to tournament games. If there's a lot of divergence (and early results in this thread suggest that this is, in fact, the case) the KRACH based monte carlo predictors will do a pretty bad job of predicting empirical reality.

One way to think about this point is to compare it to models of the presidential election (NO POLITICS - this is about modeling decisions). In 2016 a lot of models had these hugely, almost impossibly confident predictions of a Clinton victory. Several reputable polling-based predictive models had less than a 5% chance of Trump winning, while 538 had a 10% chance of Trump winning while losing the popular vote. If you look at the recaps one key reason was because those models took a fairly naive approach to modeling empirical error in polling predictors. Specifically, they failed to account for correlated polling errors across states with similar demographic characteristics. 538 took some (IMHO valid) criticism that their adjustments were being too strongly applied or that they weren't accounting for the error terms in those estimates, but in an empirical model I think that's a better class of mistake to make than just saying "this thing that happens every election doesn't happen in my model because I said so."

Back to hockey, one could imagine the same thing happening with KRACH. If KRACH systematically over-states the odds that highly rated Team A will beat lower ranked Team B, you'll get over-confident predictions for any team with a sufficiently strong record relative to its competition. Assuming jfeath17's data is correct, that is precisely what appears to be happening. This is not a flaw in KRACH, necessarily, because KRACH is meant to provide a ranked set rather than absolute determination of odds of victory. However, when using KRACH as a forward looking predictor you really do need to adjust for that variance if your model is to be empirically accurate.

This doesn't mean the pure KRACH model is useless - it's interesting, and it gives some baseline for discussion and adaptation, and it lets us talk about hockey (and math) on a Tuesday.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: abmarks (---.hsd1.ma.comcast.net)
Date: February 20, 2018 02:36PM

OK Bearlover, what you are really arguing then is that KRACH itself is worthless for any use, and you are making a completely specious argument based on your intuition, not any actual examination of data or the methods used in the CHN model about likelihood of winning the Conference tournament.

So I'll make this objective with data. I'm curious (genuinely, not snidely) whether you agree with what KRACH says for comparisons against particular opponents? I ask, because if the individual comparisons are correct, then that 59% number is correct, since it's just math at that point.

We'll use standings as of today and assume all seeds hold so as to simplify this model. We'd have the following matchups:

Quarters: (For simplicity, let's call this one a single game, not best of three.)
We play #8 Yale. Yale's KRACH is 98.3. Our KRACH is 512.9.
-This means that we are beating Yale 84% of the time. (Not sure how to do the math, but this should also imply that our chances of winning a best of three are even higher?)

Semis
We play #4 Harvard. HVD KRACH is 136.9. Our KRACH is 512.9.
-This means that we are beating HVD 79% of the time.

Championship
Champ #2 Union Union KRACH is 142.5. Our KRACH is 512.9.
-This means that we are beating Union 78% of the time.

===> Odds of winning tournament are 52%. (84% x 79% x 78%)

This is essentially what the model did that got 59%. (The difference is due to possibilities of upsets and that they ran monte carlo, not a single calculation. )

Bearlover, the question is- do you disagree with the individual KRACH comparisons?
-A

p.s. someone correct me if I got the KRACH math wrong.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 20, 2018 04:45PM

Tom Lento
Back to hockey, one could imagine the same thing happening with KRACH. If KRACH systematically over-states the odds that highly rated Team A will beat lower ranked Team B, you'll get over-confident predictions for any team with a sufficiently strong record relative to its competition. Assuming jfeath17's data is correct, that is precisely what appears to be happening. This is not a flaw in KRACH, necessarily, because KRACH is meant to provide a ranked set rather than absolute determination of odds of victory. However, when using KRACH as a forward looking predictor you really do need to adjust for that variance if your model is to be empirically accurate.

This is where I'll again repeat that - if you have a better model - please feel free to share.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 20, 2018 05:16PM

Tom Lento
One way to think about this point is to compare it to models of the presidential election (NO POLITICS - this is about modeling decisions). In 2016 a lot of models had these hugely, almost impossibly confident predictions of a Clinton victory. Several reputable polling-based predictive models had less than a 5% chance of Trump winning, while 538 had a 10% chance of Trump winning while losing the popular vote. If you look at the recaps one key reason was because those models took a fairly naive approach to modeling empirical error in polling predictors. Specifically, they failed to account for correlated polling errors across states with similar demographic characteristics. 538 took some (IMHO valid) criticism that their adjustments were being too strongly applied or that they weren't accounting for the error terms in those estimates, but in an empirical model I think that's a better class of mistake to make than just saying "this thing that happens every election doesn't happen in my model because I said so."

538's last polls only forecast was 29% Trump. And they admit that their problem was not enough late state polling. So they couldn't have seen the state results that gave him the Electoral College.

So if you don't have the data, you can't get accuracy.

"Getting back to hockey", you have the same problem. Does anyone really think you can input the data on injuries, players in the Olympics, etc.?

This whole discussion is "worthless" unless someone can put up a better way.

We all can come up with problems with the "science", but unless someone is willing to put their money where their mouth is(or fingers are), we can carry on the discussion forever without anything changing.

Finally, it's interesting all the discussions that happen once we start winning again.

For that I'm happy.

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Tom Lento (199.201.64.---)
Date: February 20, 2018 05:18PM

adamw
Tom Lento
Back to hockey, one could imagine the same thing happening with KRACH. If KRACH systematically over-states the odds that highly rated Team A will beat lower ranked Team B, you'll get over-confident predictions for any team with a sufficiently strong record relative to its competition. Assuming jfeath17's data is correct, that is precisely what appears to be happening. This is not a flaw in KRACH, necessarily, because KRACH is meant to provide a ranked set rather than absolute determination of odds of victory. However, when using KRACH as a forward looking predictor you really do need to adjust for that variance if your model is to be empirically accurate.

This is where I'll again repeat that - if you have a better model - please feel free to share.

I don't have a better model handy because making a better model requires a lot of effort and I'm not currently unemployed (or employed in a place where I get paid to do this kind of thing). If I ever take a few months off work trying to build something like this would be super fun, although as a follower of the game trying to make the advanced stats more useful for me is probably what I'd do first.

That said, you can account for the variance against empirical reality by measuring it, adding uncertainty to the model (perhaps via weighting KRACH-predicted outcomes), and backtesting to validate.

More generally, though, I think you can start simpler by seeing how far off of empirical reality the model predictions have been. If you're 95% accurate, why bother? If you're way off, how much does the model improve by adjusting each individual assumption? Does adding the error variance into the simulation help? Or is the main issue the lack of KRACH updating? Or should you adjust KRACh weight by other factors (corsi, PDO, whatever)?
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-123.myvzw.com)
Date: February 20, 2018 05:20PM

abmarks
OK Bearlover, what you are really arguing then is that KRACH itself is worthless for any use, and you are making a completely specious argument based on your intuition, not any actual examination of data or the methods used in the CHN model about likelihood of winning the Conference tournament.
Huh? KRACH is the best tool we have for ranking/seeding teams. It's just a poor predictive tool. Tom Lento explained this better than I could have, so please refer to his post. To say KRACH is not a good predictive tool because it does not account for the (significant) natural variance leading up to a specified point in a hockey season (which it shouldn't/can't, because it's not meant to be predictive) is not "specious" and is based on an "examination of the methods used in the CHN model." I'm not sure at this point what is confusing about what I am saying, to be totally honest. So, no, those underlying predictive numbers you cited are not correct, because they're taking as certain the outputs of a model that ranks teams in a very random sport based on 25-ish very random events. We are not 80% to bear Harvard and Union.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: abmarks (---.hsd1.ma.comcast.net)
Date: February 20, 2018 07:19PM

Swampy

Some things I'd want to add to the discussion:

2. Exactly how does variance play out in these methods. If Team A plays Team B, does the P[Team A or Team B wins] = 1.0? Suppose Team A has P[winning] = 0.6, and Team B has 0.4, but Team A is erratic (I'm looking at you Clarkson), while Team B is not. Does A's greater variance show up in the prediction?


Let's say we know that in the long run, A beats B 75% of the time. So, over 100 games, A wins 75.

What the P(A winning) does NOT tell you is which of those 100 games A wins. A could go 0-10, then 75-5, then 0-10 over the course of those 100.

Taking that back to the topic at hand, short term results (ie the 1 game result in a tournament) are going to vary a lot vs. the long-term percentage.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 21, 2018 12:13PM

Tom Lento
That said, you can account for the variance against empirical reality by measuring it, adding uncertainty to the model (perhaps via weighting KRACH-predicted outcomes), and backtesting to validate.

More generally, though, I think you can start simpler by seeing how far off of empirical reality the model predictions have been. If you're 95% accurate, why bother? If you're way off, how much does the model improve by adjusting each individual assumption? Does adding the error variance into the simulation help? Or is the main issue the lack of KRACH updating? Or should you adjust KRACh weight by other factors (corsi, PDO, whatever)?

If I had any idea how to do this correctly, it would already have been done.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: upprdeck (---.fs.cornell.edu)
Date: February 21, 2018 12:32PM

if you can improve the model lets get it working for horse racing as that would provide you the time to tweak the hockey model once we get rich..
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Tom Lento (199.201.64.---)
Date: February 21, 2018 04:11PM

Jim Hyla
Tom Lento
One way to think about this point is to compare it to models of the presidential election (NO POLITICS - this is about modeling decisions). In 2016 a lot of models had these hugely, almost impossibly confident predictions of a Clinton victory. Several reputable polling-based predictive models had less than a 5% chance of Trump winning, while 538 had a 10% chance of Trump winning while losing the popular vote. If you look at the recaps one key reason was because those models took a fairly naive approach to modeling empirical error in polling predictors. Specifically, they failed to account for correlated polling errors across states with similar demographic characteristics. 538 took some (IMHO valid) criticism that their adjustments were being too strongly applied or that they weren't accounting for the error terms in those estimates, but in an empirical model I think that's a better class of mistake to make than just saying "this thing that happens every election doesn't happen in my model because I said so."

538's last polls only forecast was 29% Trump. And they admit that their problem was not enough late state polling. So they couldn't have seen the state results that gave him the Electoral College.

So if you don't have the data, you can't get accuracy.

"Getting back to hockey", you have the same problem. Does anyone really think you can input the data on injuries, players in the Olympics, etc.?

This whole discussion is "worthless" unless someone can put up a better way.

We all can come up with problems with the "science", but unless someone is willing to put their money where their mouth is(or fingers are), we can carry on the discussion forever without anything changing.

Finally, it's interesting all the discussions that happen once we start winning again.

For that I'm happy.

Note all of the caveats in my statement about 538s prediction. Their model gave Trump a 10% chance of winning the election while losing the popular vote, and a 29% chance of winning overall. The Princeton Election Consortium gave Trump a 2% chance of winning the election at all at one point late in the race.

The way 538 approaches these problems is to tune a model based on parameters that explain variance from empirical reality, and then to back-test that model against the actual results. If you look at their CARMElo ratings for the NBA they basically incorporate factors which might contribute to fatigue and injury (travel, back to back games) in ways that empirically affect performance, without worrying as much about whether or not Steph Curry the individual player will get hurt.

The same thing applies in hockey. The fact that the variance in single game outcomes is larger in hockey than basketball makes the problem harder, of course.

I can't really tell Adam exactly how to do this - I'm not that familiar with monte carlo and I don't have enough direct experience in this domain to do more than provide vague suggestions, and unless I take the time to get my hands dirty with it I won't be able to speak intelligently about which approaches to consider and which to discard.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Tom Lento (199.201.64.---)
Date: February 21, 2018 04:48PM

adamw
Tom Lento
That said, you can account for the variance against empirical reality by measuring it, adding uncertainty to the model (perhaps via weighting KRACH-predicted outcomes), and backtesting to validate.

More generally, though, I think you can start simpler by seeing how far off of empirical reality the model predictions have been. If you're 95% accurate, why bother? If you're way off, how much does the model improve by adjusting each individual assumption? Does adding the error variance into the simulation help? Or is the main issue the lack of KRACH updating? Or should you adjust KRACh weight by other factors (corsi, PDO, whatever)?

If I had any idea how to do this correctly, it would already have been done.

Yeah, that's the hard part. There's a ton of literature on evaluating predictive models, but I haven't done anything even adjacent to this field for years so I wouldn't know what to recommend as an intro. When my work involved statistical modeling it wasn't in these domains anyway, so I don't have answers off the top of my head either. :(

Just to be clear, I like the models, and it's fun (for me, at least) to think of alternatives. If I stumble across a relevant approach for you I'll pass it along. Thanks for putting them up for us! :)
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 21, 2018 06:13PM

The hard part is everyone complaining about what is out there, including Adam's, and no one has an answer, or is willing to help.

I think it's a lot of fun to look at these models, but I don't have a clue about what to do (easily) to improve them, so I enjoy what's there and keep my mouth shut about complaining they aren't good enough.

It seems we go through this every spring. At least every spring where it means something to us and the post-season.

Adam takes a lot of crap for no good reason, he's trying a lot harder than many others.

Now if he could only fix the app on my iPhone, so it wouldn't screw up so often, that would be nice.........:-Dbolt

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 21, 2018 07:32PM

Jim Hyla
The hard part is everyone complaining about what is out there, including Adam's, and no one has an answer, or is willing to help.

I think it's a lot of fun to look at these models, but I don't have a clue about what to do (easily) to improve them, so I enjoy what's there and keep my mouth shut about complaining they aren't good enough.

It seems we go through this every spring. At least every spring where it means something to us and the post-season.

Adam takes a lot of crap for no good reason, he's trying a lot harder than many others.

Now if he could only fix the app on my iPhone, so it wouldn't screw up so often, that would be nice.........:-Dbolt
I have nothing against Adam and I love CHN. But that doesn't mean we should be quiet about predictions that are based on flawed assumptions. I also think it's better to have no prediction model at all than to have one that is based on flawed assumptions. Coverage of the 2016 election would have been vastly improved had flawed models like HuffPost's not existed. America would have known that for almost the entirety of the race Hillary was only a slight favorite, that the electoral college favored Trump, that Comey's letter very likely cost Clinton the election. Instead, the media, in part because of models like HuffPost's and others', covered Hillary's victory as a foregone conclusion. Obviously the stakes aren't as high here, but no one is helped by a model that wrongly portrays Cornell's odds against Union as 80%, or its odds of winning the ECAC as 60%.
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/21/2018 07:33PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Swampy (---.ri.ri.cox.net)
Date: February 22, 2018 10:23AM

abmarks
Swampy

Some things I'd want to add to the discussion:

2. Exactly how does variance play out in these methods. If Team A plays Team B, does the P[Team A or Team B wins] = 1.0? Suppose Team A has P[winning] = 0.6, and Team B has 0.4, but Team A is erratic (I'm looking at you Clarkson), while Team B is not. Does A's greater variance show up in the prediction?


Let's say we know that in the long run, A beats B 75% of the time. So, over 100 games, A wins 75.

What the P(A winning) does NOT tell you is which of those 100 games A wins. A could go 0-10, then 75-5, then 0-10 over the course of those 100.

Taking that back to the topic at hand, short term results (ie the 1 game result in a tournament) are going to vary a lot vs. the long-term percentage.

I understand this but was talking about variance in several other senses. I'll explain them here. WARNING: THE FOLLOWING IS QUITE WONKISH.


Assumptions

Assume two teams, Team C and Team H, belong to a 12-team league in which teams play each other twice during the season. So each team plays 22 league games. Also assume teams earn 0 points in the league standings for a loss, 1 for a tie, and 2 for a win.



Estimation Variance

For the moment, ignore ties. Any data-based estimate of a team's chances of winning a game can be thought of as a function. If pC is the probability Team C wins a game, then let pC be the estimate of that probability. So that:

(1) pC = f(data)

In other words, the estimated probability is a function of whatever data are used in the estimate. When we say "data," this includes the number of data points (sample size) used to make the estimate.

Now, if we know the mathematical properties of f() we may be able to derive, mathematically, an expression for the variance of pC, var(pC). Call this the estimation variance, a measure if the estimate's the precision.

If we do not know the estimating function's mathematical properties, we still may be able to estimate var(pC) using simulation and resampling techniques.



Game and Game-Series Estimates


Think of a single game as an experiment with two possible outcomes: "success" and "failure." For simplicity, assume we actually know the real probability of each, so we don't have to use estimates like (1). To think about this, just consider Team C for now.

Let:

p = probability Team C wins
q = probability Team C loses = 1 - p

Furthermore, to convert the results into a number, define a random variable, X = 1 for a win and 0 for a loss. This is well known as a Bernoulli Trial, and X has a Bernoulli Distribution. The variance of X is given by:

(2) var(X) = pq

In the present context, call this "game variance" since it is the variance related to the outcome of a single game.


We can also think of a "series variance", which is the variance associated with a team winning a series. To simplify the math, let's disregard the fact that some series end after a team has won the majority of games in the series (e.g., 2 out of 3), and just think of the number of wins in a series. Define a second random variable, Yn as the number of wins in a series of n games. If each game has the same probabilities of its outcome, then Yn is the sum of n X's. In other words, it has a binomial distribution, the variance of which equals:

(3) var(Yn) = np(1-p)

In both (2) and (3) the variance depends on the value of p. If p = 0, the variance is 0, and similarly for p = 1. The variance is at its maximum, 0.25, when p = 0.5.

It's important to note here that the variance depends on the underlying, real probabilities and is not a matter of estimation.


Comments on jfeath17's chart

  1. The chart shows a relation between the Krach and actual game outcomes. Because of the properties of Bernoulli and Binomial distributions, the variance necessarily decreases as p moves away from 0.5 and closer to 1.0. So we would expect better predictions to the right of the graph. But the graph is almost a straight line up to about p = 0.85 and then drops off slightly. Maybe this is due to a weakness in the Krach, which is not intended to predict outcomes. Or maybe that's why they play the game.
  2. The chart would be improved with confidence bands, which are sensitive to variance and graphically show how confident one should be about the fitted line.
Notice though that confidence intervals plotted around a curve like this, which is based on empirical data, themselves estimated from other data (as in Equation 1), have two sources of variance: Estimation Variance and Game variance.



Perfomance Variance

In addition to the above, we should consider the variance of a given team's performance. Some teams are reliable; others are erratic. This can be best explained with an example.

Suppose every one of the 10 "other" teams always scores exactly 3 goals in every game. Then if O is the number of goals one of these "other" teams scores, the expected number of goals is 3 (E[O] = 3), and the variance is zero (var[O] = 0).

Similarly, assume Team C always scores 4 goals when it plays. Then if C is a random variable equal to the number of goals Team C scores, E[C] = 4 and var[C] = 0.

We can see right away that over the season Team C will always win over the ten "other" teams, so just from playing them it will accumulate 40 points (10 teams, 2 games per team, 2 points per win).


But now consider Team H, which is more erratic. Let H be the number of goals it scores in any given game. Like Team C let Team H's expected number of goals be 4: E[H] = 4. But unlike Team C, var[H] will not be zero.

Instead, suppose H has the following probability mass distribution: P[H = 2] = 0.10, P[H = 3] = 0.15, P[H = 4] = 0.50, P[H = 5] = 0.15, and P[H = 6] = 0.10. So here we can see different results when Team H plays its 20 games against the 10 "other" teams: the expected number of losses is 2, the expected number of ties is 3, and the expected number of wins is 15. So when Team H plays the other teams, the expected number of points is only 33, unlike Team C's 40!

What about when Team C and Team H play each other? Even though both have the same expected number of goals, the variance of Team H means it will be expected to lose to Team C 25% of the time, tie Team C 50% of the time, and beat Team C 25% of the time. In each of their 2 games against each other during the regular season, 2 points is at stake. So Team H can expect 0.5 points from a tie (1 point x 0.5 probability) and 0.5 points from a win (2 points x 0.25 probability), or 1 point in total. Similarly for Team C. With both teams playing each other twice during the season, each expects to get 2 points. This makes sense, because they're evenly matched.

But in terms of total points in the league, Team C expects to have 42 points at season's end, but Team H expects only 35 points. Which is how things should be, because Team H sucks.

Notice here that the only difference between the two teams is their respective variances, but it makes a big difference. If we look more closely at games against the 10 "other" teams, we are much more confident that Team C will beat them, whereas we expect Team H to lose to some of them. This is why performance variance is also important in thinking about which teams are likely to win particular games. Again, here there's no estimation issue. We know what the probabilities really are, yet variance affects the outcome.



Technical Suggestion

Jfeath17 asked for suggestions regarding the graphical analysis. For this kind of work I highly recommend the R Project's free, open-source statistical software used in conjunction with the RStudio GUI interface. It would allow easy addition of things like confidence bands in the probability plots, weighting of recent time-series data, etc.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: nshapiro (192.148.195.---)
Date: February 22, 2018 11:39AM

H sucks...sorry wrote this before I read your whole post
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/22/2018 11:45AM by nshapiro.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Trotsky (---.dc.dc.cox.net)
Date: February 22, 2018 11:54AM

nshapiro
H sucks..
I believe you are referring to Team A.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jeff Hopkins '82 (---.37.26.69.virtela.net)
Date: February 22, 2018 11:57AM

Trotsky
nshapiro
H sucks..
I believe you are referring to Team A.

The party of the first part, hereafter referred to as the "Party of the First Part"...

No, I don't like that part.

Which part?

The first part.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Trotsky (---.dc.dc.cox.net)
Date: February 22, 2018 12:03PM

Jeff Hopkins '82
Trotsky
nshapiro
H sucks..
I believe you are referring to Team A.

The party of the first part, hereafter referred to as the "Party of the First Part"...

No, I don't like that part.

Which part?

The first part.

You can't fool me. There is no sanity clause.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: scoop85 (---.nyc.biz.rr.com)
Date: February 22, 2018 12:37PM

All I can say is I'm glad I never took statistics!
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: KGR11 (---.nycmny.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 22, 2018 02:15PM

jfeath17, do you have an equation for that logistic regression? If you do, adamw could plug the KRACH-generated winning percentage into it get an empirical winning % for the pairwise probability matrix. adamw, for the Monte Carlo with 20k samples, I estimate this will add a couple million extra computations to the model. I'm pretty sure that's not a big deal.

There may be confidence questions with jfeath17's model (ideally, we'd want more than 2 years of data), but it answers the question that's been raised: it takes the KRACH reported winning percentage and turns into the winning percentage that actually happened.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jeff Hopkins '82 (---.102.128.104.res-cmts.sm.ptd.net)
Date: February 23, 2018 06:27AM

Trotsky
Jeff Hopkins '82
Trotsky
nshapiro
H sucks..
I believe you are referring to Team A.

The party of the first part, hereafter referred to as the "Party of the First Part"...

No, I don't like that part.

Which part?

The first part.

You can't fool me. There is no sanity clause.

+1
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 25, 2018 02:09AM

Jim Hyla
Adam takes a lot of crap for no good reason, he's trying a lot harder than many others.

Thank you, sir.

Jim Hyla
Now if he could only fix the app on my iPhone, so it wouldn't screw up so often, that would be nice.........:-Dbolt

For the record, the correct person to address this issue to is much closer to you, vis-a-vis this forum, than I am. :)
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 25, 2018 02:15AM

Swampy
....

But now consider Team H, which is more erratic. Let H be the number of goals it scores in any given game. Like Team C let Team H's expected number of goals be 4: E[H] = 4. But unlike Team C, var[H] will not be zero.

Instead, suppose H has the following probability mass distribution: P[H = 2] = 0.10, P[H = 3] = 0.15, P[H = 4] = 0.50, P[H = 5] = 0.15, and P[H = 6] = 0.10. So here we can see different results when Team H plays its 20 games against the 10 "other" teams: the expected number of losses is 2, the expected number of ties is 3, and the expected number of wins is 15. So when Team H plays the other teams, the expected number of points is only 33, unlike Team C's 40!
....

I like where all this headed. I just don't know how to implement. But philosophically, it looks sound to me. Anything added to the model should be based on sound statistical principle like this that is repeatable and set, and not vague "tweaking" based on assumptions.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 25, 2018 02:16AM

BearLover
I have nothing against Adam and I love CHN. But that doesn't mean we should be quiet about predictions that are based on flawed assumptions. I also think it's better to have no prediction model at all than to have one that is based on flawed assumptions. Coverage of the 2016 election would have been vastly improved had flawed models like HuffPost's not existed. America would have known that for almost the entirety of the race Hillary was only a slight favorite, that the electoral college favored Trump, that Comey's letter very likely cost Clinton the election. Instead, the media, in part because of models like HuffPost's and others', covered Hillary's victory as a foregone conclusion. Obviously the stakes aren't as high here, but no one is helped by a model that wrongly portrays Cornell's odds against Union as 80%, or its odds of winning the ECAC as 60%.

Thanks - and feel free to complain all you want. But I disagree the model is flawed. It might be incomplete, but I wouldn't call it flawed. There's nothing flawed about how the KRACH computes itself. The non-538 presidential race models were flawed, because they made really poor assumptions.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 25, 2018 09:56AM

adamw
Jim Hyla
Now if he could only fix the app on my iPhone, so it wouldn't screw up so often, that would be nice.........:-Dbolt

For the record, the correct person to address this issue to is much closer to you, vis-a-vis this forum, than I am. :)

I know that, but I had to say something bad, didn't I?nut

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: billhoward (---.nwrk.east.verizon.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 01:07AM

ECAC end of 2017-18 regular season
1. Cornell
2. Union
3, Clarkson
4. Harvard
5. (t) Dartmouth gets 12 seed first round
5. (t) Colgate gets 11 seed
7. Princeton
8. Yale
9. Quinnipiac
10. Brown
11. Rensselaer
12. St. Lawrence

After the first round (where 1-4 have a bye) the tournament is reseeded. We get the lowest survivor. If higher seeds win the first and second weeks, we'd play 8 Yale in Ithaca and then 4 Harvard Friday (early game) in Lake Placid. What is the likeliest first round upset?
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.ri.ri.cox.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 12:39PM

billhoward
What is the likeliest first round upset?

Brown or RPI (are tied for "likeliest";).
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Trotsky (---.dc.dc.cox.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 12:43PM

8. Yale
9. Quinnipiac
10. Brown
11. RPI
12. St. Lawrence

We will face one of these teams in the QF.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: marty (---.sub-70-209-169.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 12:54PM

Swampy
billhoward
What is the likeliest first round upset?

Brown or RPI (are tied for "likeliest";).

If RPI beats Colton Point, I'll donate the refund they owe me for first round tickets. But I'll donate it to our Onion bruised Pep Band.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: KGR11 (---.nycmny.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 01:05PM

Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: CU2007 (---.sub-70-214-83.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 01:22PM

KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).

Clarkson has won 2 of their last 12 games. I’d give Harvard, Union and maybe even Colgate a better shot at winning the tournament.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.163.128.131.dhcp.uri.edu)
Date: February 26, 2018 02:03PM

CU2007
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).

Clarkson has won 2 of their last 12 games. I’d give Harvard, Union and maybe even Colgate a better shot at winning the tournament.

Let's hope we never can prove you're right.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-117.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 02:29PM

KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/26/2018 02:30PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: ugarte (---.177.169.163.ipyx-102276-zyo.zip.zayo.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 02:44PM

BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.

 
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-117.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 03:12PM

ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Dafatone (---.southeasttech.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 03:39PM

BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

It's perfectly fine for a model that doesn't bother with recent trends.

Which is a big weakness, sure, but that doesn't mean it's necessarily worth tossing out.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 03:41PM

BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

Why? If someone wants to discuss something that you think is faulty, why do you think you have the right to tell them to stop.

You have the right to point out that you think it's faulty, YOU DO NOT HAVE THE ABILITY TO MAKE THEM STOP.

THEY ARE NOT HURTING YOU. PLEASE LEAVE THEM ALONE AND LET THEM HAVE THEIR OWN FUN.

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-117.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 03:59PM

Dafatone
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

It's perfectly fine for a model that doesn't bother with recent trends.

Which is a big weakness, sure, but that doesn't mean it's necessarily worth tossing out.
The main critique of the model has nothing to do with it not accounting for recent trends--scroll up for a longer explanation, but the most glaring problem with the model is that it assumes the #3 team in the KRACH/PWR/RPI is the *actual* third-best team in the country, despite the incredibly high amount of randomness in a 30-game season. I believe the model is assigning Cornell (and everyone else) win probabilities based off them being ranked X after an infinite number or games rather than after 29 games. There was a study some number of years ago that found that several thousands of baseball games (way more than just a 162-game season) are necessary to determine who the best team is. The study answered a bit of a different question in a different sport, but the idea is the same: there is way too much randomness in a small season to say with a high degree of certainty how good a team really is.
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/26/2018 04:09PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-117.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 04:04PM

Jim Hyla
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

Why? If someone wants to discuss something that you think is faulty, why do you think you have the right to tell them to stop.

You have the right to point out that you think it's faulty, YOU DO NOT HAVE THE ABILITY TO MAKE THEM STOP.

THEY ARE NOT HURTING YOU. PLEASE LEAVE THEM ALONE AND LET THEM HAVE THEIR OWN FUN.
The capital letters in my post were supposed to be funny...
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Trotsky (---.dc.dc.cox.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 04:10PM

BearLover
Dafatone
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

It's perfectly fine for a model that doesn't bother with recent trends.

Which is a big weakness, sure, but that doesn't mean it's necessarily worth tossing out.
The main critique of the model has nothing to do with it not accounting for recent trends--scroll up for a longer explanation, but the most glaring problem with the model is that it assumes the #3 team in the KRACH/PWR/RPI is the *actual* third-best team in the country, despite the incredibly high amount of randomness in a 30-game season. I believe the model is assigning Cornell (and everyone else) win probabilities based off them being ranked X after an infinite number or games rather than after 29 games. There was a study some number of years ago that found that several thousands of baseball games (way more than just a 162-game season) are necessary to determine who the best team is. The study answered a bit of a different question in a different sport, but the idea is the same: there is way too much randomness in a small season to say with a high degree of certainty who the best teams are.

An entire branch of mathematics has been beavering away at this ever since Laplace and Gauss. There are tools to determine the actual quality of models. I've seen nobody post any objective evaluation of the model in question.

Until somebody does the math this is just somebody saying "well that doesn't feel right." Math doesn't give a shit about your feelings. Do the work, cite somebody who does, or go to the water wings side of the pool.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-117.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 04:21PM

Trotsky
BearLover
Dafatone
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

It's perfectly fine for a model that doesn't bother with recent trends.

Which is a big weakness, sure, but that doesn't mean it's necessarily worth tossing out.
The main critique of the model has nothing to do with it not accounting for recent trends--scroll up for a longer explanation, but the most glaring problem with the model is that it assumes the #3 team in the KRACH/PWR/RPI is the *actual* third-best team in the country, despite the incredibly high amount of randomness in a 30-game season. I believe the model is assigning Cornell (and everyone else) win probabilities based off them being ranked X after an infinite number or games rather than after 29 games. There was a study some number of years ago that found that several thousands of baseball games (way more than just a 162-game season) are necessary to determine who the best team is. The study answered a bit of a different question in a different sport, but the idea is the same: there is way too much randomness in a small season to say with a high degree of certainty who the best teams are.

An entire branch of mathematics has been beavering away at this ever since Laplace and Gauss. There are tools to determine the actual quality of models. I've seen nobody post any objective evaluation of the model in question.

Until somebody does the math this is just somebody saying "well that doesn't feel right." Math doesn't give a shit about your feelings. Do the work, cite somebody who does, or go to the water wings side of the pool.
jfeath17 did some math earlier in this thread, you should check it out
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.163.128.131.dhcp.uri.edu)
Date: February 26, 2018 04:49PM

Trotsky
Math doesn't give a shit about your feelings. ...

How do you know this? Do you have some sort of mathematical model that you've tested with empirical data? demented
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: imafrshmn (---.hsd1.mi.comcast.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 04:49PM

BearLover
Trotsky
BearLover
Dafatone
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

It's perfectly fine for a model that doesn't bother with recent trends.

Which is a big weakness, sure, but that doesn't mean it's necessarily worth tossing out.
The main critique of the model has nothing to do with it not accounting for recent trends--scroll up for a longer explanation, but the most glaring problem with the model is that it assumes the #3 team in the KRACH/PWR/RPI is the *actual* third-best team in the country, despite the incredibly high amount of randomness in a 30-game season. I believe the model is assigning Cornell (and everyone else) win probabilities based off them being ranked X after an infinite number or games rather than after 29 games. There was a study some number of years ago that found that several thousands of baseball games (way more than just a 162-game season) are necessary to determine who the best team is. The study answered a bit of a different question in a different sport, but the idea is the same: there is way too much randomness in a small season to say with a high degree of certainty who the best teams are.

An entire branch of mathematics has been beavering away at this ever since Laplace and Gauss. There are tools to determine the actual quality of models. I've seen nobody post any objective evaluation of the model in question.

Until somebody does the math this is just somebody saying "well that doesn't feel right." Math doesn't give a shit about your feelings. Do the work, cite somebody who does, or go to the water wings side of the pool.
jfeath17 did some math earlier in this thread, you should check it out

jfeath17’s curve is interesting, but what we really need to do is figure out a way to estimate, for each team, the variance of KRACH as a function of results and number of games played. If we’re going to assume gaussian distributions, we may need to work with a log-transformed KRACH value. This would allow us to make a meaningful improvement of the CHN prediction model without throwing out the basic structure of it. If i had a lot of free time, i would be willing to work on this, but i’m pretty busy.

 
___________________________
class of '09

Edited 3 time(s). Last edit at 02/26/2018 05:03PM by imafrshmn.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 04:57PM

BearLover
Jim Hyla
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

Why? If someone wants to discuss something that you think is faulty, why do you think you have the right to tell them to stop.

You have the right to point out that you think it's faulty, YOU DO NOT HAVE THE ABILITY TO MAKE THEM STOP.

THEY ARE NOT HURTING YOU. PLEASE LEAVE THEM ALONE AND LET THEM HAVE THEIR OWN FUN.
The capital letters in my post were supposed to be funny...

Really, you're not kidding me now, are you. Based upon your past posts, I would never expect you to be funny about this. It's hard for me to believe, but if you say so, I'll go with it. You should learn to use proper emojis, that's what they're for. You see, we can't see your facial expression when you're posting.

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.sub-70-214-117.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 05:03PM

Jim Hyla
BearLover
Jim Hyla
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

Why? If someone wants to discuss something that you think is faulty, why do you think you have the right to tell them to stop.

You have the right to point out that you think it's faulty, YOU DO NOT HAVE THE ABILITY TO MAKE THEM STOP.

THEY ARE NOT HURTING YOU. PLEASE LEAVE THEM ALONE AND LET THEM HAVE THEIR OWN FUN.
The capital letters in my post were supposed to be funny...

Really, you're not kidding me now, are you. Based upon your past posts, I would never expect you to be funny about this. It's hard for me to believe, but if you say so, I'll go with it. You should learn to use proper emojis, that's what they're for. You see, we can't see your facial expression when you're posting.
The caps were supposed to indicate playful yelling. I've noticed a lot of hostility being read into my posts here but I guess that comes with the territory of disagreeing with everybody.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Jim Hyla (---.239.191.68.cl.cstel.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 05:22PM

BearLover
Jim Hyla
BearLover
Jim Hyla
BearLover
ugarte
BearLover
KGR11
Updated probabilities for ECAC teams with byes to win the ECAC championship from the pairwise probability matrix:
Cornell: 55%
Clarkson: 22%
Union: 10%
Harvard: 6%

The average team in the quarterfinal has a 12.5% probability to win the championship; Cornell is 4.4x more likely than the average QF team to win it all. That's a testament how great Cornell's season has been (or how bad the season's been for the rest of the ECAC).
It's also a testament to how bad this prediction model is. STOP CITING THIS PREDICTION MODEL
please take a xanax. i also think the prediction model wildly overstates our chances but it literally doesn't matter.
Every sports discussion literally doesn't matter, but if we're going to discuss sports we could at least stop basing those discussions off the same faulty model that people keep citing every five minutes.

Why? If someone wants to discuss something that you think is faulty, why do you think you have the right to tell them to stop.

You have the right to point out that you think it's faulty, YOU DO NOT HAVE THE ABILITY TO MAKE THEM STOP.

THEY ARE NOT HURTING YOU. PLEASE LEAVE THEM ALONE AND LET THEM HAVE THEIR OWN FUN.
The capital letters in my post were supposed to be funny...

Really, you're not kidding me now, are you. Based upon your past posts, I would never expect you to be funny about this. It's hard for me to believe, but if you say so, I'll go with it. You should learn to use proper emojis, that's what they're for. You see, we can't see your facial expression when you're posting.
The caps were supposed to indicate playful yelling. I've noticed a lot of hostility being read into my posts here but I guess that comes with the territory of disagreeing with everybody.

I'll say it again, if after being so negative on a point, you mean to be funny, then you need to make it clear.

Second, a large part of the "hostility" is your insistence that people who want to use a particular model stop doing it.

You've made your point that you don't feel it's valid. So if it makes someone else feel good to use it, LET IT GO (and I'm not trying to be funny). Let them have their fun. What harm does it do?

 
___________________________
"Cornell Fans Made the Timbers Tremble", Boston Globe, March/1970
Cornell lawyers stopped the candy throwing. Jan/2005
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: KGR11 (---.sub-70-214-110.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 06:44PM

My purpose in posting the percentages was to add new context to the discussion of KRACH's AQ results. BearLover doesn't buy the probability attributed to Cornell to win the AQ and I wanted to outline how it compares to other bye teams' AQ probabilities. I don't think this changes anyone's opinion, but it's an interesting metric to show how KRACH judges the top 4 ECAC teams.
Of course, part of the reason Cornell's probability is so high is that they face the easiest path as far as ECAC standings go. They'd be less likely to win if the tournament didn't reseed (I believe KRACH and intuition agree on this).
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Jeff Hopkins '82 (---.102.128.104.res-cmts.sm.ptd.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 07:12PM

Can we please create a separate thread for people to argue about mathematical models and statistics?
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 26, 2018 07:16PM

KGR11
My purpose in posting the percentages was to add new context to the discussion of KRACH's AQ results. BearLover doesn't buy the probability attributed to Cornell to win the AQ and I wanted to outline how it compares to other bye teams' AQ probabilities. I don't think this changes anyone's opinion, but it's an interesting metric to show how KRACH judges the top 4 ECAC teams.
Of course, part of the reason Cornell's probability is so high is that they face the easiest path as far as ECAC standings go. They'd be less likely to win if the tournament didn't reseed (I believe KRACH and intuition agree on this).
Okay, that's fair, and you make a good point about the last part. Though assuming there is no more than one upset in the first round, I don't think we have an easier path in the quarters than any of the other bye teams, since we get Q/Y. If one of the top four gets upset in the quarters, though, we would have an easier path.

In our six games this year against Union, Clarkson, and Harvard, we have a -1 goal differential (discounting Angello's empty-netter on the road vs. Harvard), and a -20 shot differential. We went 3-2-1 in those games, and each of those three teams have good goalies. So it's hard to figure we are more than a slight favorite against any of them.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Dafatone (---.sub-174-217-29.myvzw.com)
Date: February 26, 2018 07:42PM

BearLover
KGR11
My purpose in posting the percentages was to add new context to the discussion of KRACH's AQ results. BearLover doesn't buy the probability attributed to Cornell to win the AQ and I wanted to outline how it compares to other bye teams' AQ probabilities. I don't think this changes anyone's opinion, but it's an interesting metric to show how KRACH judges the top 4 ECAC teams.
Of course, part of the reason Cornell's probability is so high is that they face the easiest path as far as ECAC standings go. They'd be less likely to win if the tournament didn't reseed (I believe KRACH and intuition agree on this).
Okay, that's fair, and you make a good point about the last part. Though assuming there is no more than one upset in the first round, I don't think we have an easier path in the quarters than any of the other bye teams, since we get Q/Y. If one of the top four gets upset in the quarters, though, we would have an easier path.

In our six games this year against Union, Clarkson, and Harvard, we have a -1 goal differential (discounting Angello's empty-netter on the road vs. Harvard), and a -20 shot differential. We went 3-2-1 in those games, and each of those three teams have good goalies. So it's hard to figure we are more than a slight favorite against any of them.

Or to look at it through a different lens, we're 3-1 vs Harvard and union with the only loss being by one goal on the road. We're 0-1-1 vs Clarkson with that loss being fairly lopsided, but Clarkson has fallen off a cliff.

That's not to guarantee anything, of course.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: andyw2100 (---.twcny.res.rr.com)
Date: February 27, 2018 04:51PM

BearLover
Though assuming there is no more than one upset in the first round, I don't think we have an easier path in the quarters than any of the other bye teams, since we get Q/Y.

That last part isn't correct. We get the lowest remaining seed. There could be just one upset--say St. Lawrence over Dartmouth or RPI over Colgate--in which case we would play St. Lawrence or RPI.
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/27/2018 04:53PM by andyw2100.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: February 27, 2018 07:31PM

andyw2100
BearLover
Though assuming there is no more than one upset in the first round, I don't think we have an easier path in the quarters than any of the other bye teams, since we get Q/Y.

That last part isn't correct. We get the lowest remaining seed. There could be just one upset--say St. Lawrence over Dartmouth or RPI over Colgate--in which case we would play St. Lawrence or RPI.
doh
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: February 28, 2018 10:43AM

The beauty of it is, what anyone thinks of Cornell's chances vs. one team or another doesn't matter. KRACH is what it is. And there is nothing better that perfectly captures PAST results. There are many "flaws" if you will to the model when it comes to projecting odds of winning future games - but I don't think they're flaws. They're just incomplete. All of the reasons stated are valid. But, as someone said, you'd need to come to the issue with actual valid math, and a better algorithm, before getting all hot and bothered about it. Until then, saying that you "feel" that 52% chance is "flawed" is just as "flawed" of an argument as anything else.

Polls are a shit-ton more flawed than this is - doesn't stop Jim from posting them :) ... I've given him grief about it in the past, but all in fun. Would never tell him to stop.

Feel free to point out things all you want. But until you have a better model, and are willing to program it, then jeebus h. criminy, let people discuss it. It does a pretty fair job of giving you a portrait of what could happen. I think everyone here (unlike many other places) is smart enough to know to take it with some grain of salt. But it's as good a guideline as you've got.
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 02/28/2018 10:48AM by adamw.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: andyw2100 (---.twcny.res.rr.com)
Date: February 28, 2018 12:00PM

BearLover
andyw2100
BearLover
Though assuming there is no more than one upset in the first round, I don't think we have an easier path in the quarters than any of the other bye teams, since we get Q/Y.

That last part isn't correct. We get the lowest remaining seed. There could be just one upset--say St. Lawrence over Dartmouth or RPI over Colgate--in which case we would play St. Lawrence or RPI.
doh

I can't tell if the head-smack is for me, because I'm missing something, or if it's an acknowledgment of the fact that you weren't thinking completely clearly when you made the initial post.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: ugarte (---.177.169.163.ipyx-102276-zyo.zip.zayo.com)
Date: February 28, 2018 12:44PM

andyw2100
BearLover
andyw2100
BearLover
Though assuming there is no more than one upset in the first round, I don't think we have an easier path in the quarters than any of the other bye teams, since we get Q/Y.

That last part isn't correct. We get the lowest remaining seed. There could be just one upset--say St. Lawrence over Dartmouth or RPI over Colgate--in which case we would play St. Lawrence or RPI.
doh

I can't tell if the head-smack is for me, because I'm missing something, or if it's an acknowledgment of the fact that you weren't thinking completely clearly when you made the initial post.
Decide if you are right or not and give BearLover credit for the appropriate response.

 
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: KGR11 (---.nycmny.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 01, 2018 12:47PM

adamw
The beauty of it is, what anyone thinks of Cornell's chances vs. one team or another doesn't matter. KRACH is what it is. And there is nothing better that perfectly captures PAST results. There are many "flaws" if you will to the model when it comes to projecting odds of winning future games - but I don't think they're flaws. They're just incomplete. All of the reasons stated are valid. But, as someone said, you'd need to come to the issue with actual valid math, and a better algorithm, before getting all hot and bothered about it. Until then, saying that you "feel" that 52% chance is "flawed" is just as "flawed" of an argument as anything else.

Polls are a shit-ton more flawed than this is - doesn't stop Jim from posting them :) ... I've given him grief about it in the past, but all in fun. Would never tell him to stop.

Feel free to point out things all you want. But until you have a better model, and are willing to program it, then jeebus h. criminy, let people discuss it. It does a pretty fair job of giving you a portrait of what could happen. I think everyone here (unlike many other places) is smart enough to know to take it with some grain of salt. But it's as good a guideline as you've got.

Agreed. KRACH does an awesome job of ranking teams based on games played. For forecasting games, I think jfeath17's work could take it to the next level. In her logistic regression, the closer the KRACH winning percentage is to 100, the greater the difference between the KRACH winning percentage and the outcome winning percentage. I think this makes a lot of sense: if a team has a perfect record, they can still lose future games (example: 2007 Patriots and 2015 Kentucky basketball), so a team with a nearly perfect record should also have a lower probability of winning future games.

I think the biggest challenge for jfeath17 is that there's only 2 years of data. I think the next step is gather data for a couple of years and see how stable that logistic regression is year-to-year. jfeath17, I'd be interested to see a more detailed procedure that you used. That way, if you ended up stepping back, someone else could try this.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.163.128.131.dhcp.uri.edu)
Date: March 01, 2018 02:12PM

KGR11
adamw
The beauty of it is, what anyone thinks of Cornell's chances vs. one team or another doesn't matter. KRACH is what it is. And there is nothing better that perfectly captures PAST results. There are many "flaws" if you will to the model when it comes to projecting odds of winning future games - but I don't think they're flaws. They're just incomplete. All of the reasons stated are valid. But, as someone said, you'd need to come to the issue with actual valid math, and a better algorithm, before getting all hot and bothered about it. Until then, saying that you "feel" that 52% chance is "flawed" is just as "flawed" of an argument as anything else.

Polls are a shit-ton more flawed than this is - doesn't stop Jim from posting them :) ... I've given him grief about it in the past, but all in fun. Would never tell him to stop.

Feel free to point out things all you want. But until you have a better model, and are willing to program it, then jeebus h. criminy, let people discuss it. It does a pretty fair job of giving you a portrait of what could happen. I think everyone here (unlike many other places) is smart enough to know to take it with some grain of salt. But it's as good a guideline as you've got.

Agreed. KRACH does an awesome job of ranking teams based on games played. For forecasting games, I think jfeath17's work could take it to the next level. In her logistic regression, the closer the KRACH winning percentage is to 100, the greater the difference between the KRACH winning percentage and the outcome winning percentage. I think this makes a lot of sense: if a team has a perfect record, they can still lose future games (example: 2007 Patriots and 2015 Kentucky basketball), so a team with a nearly perfect record should also have a lower probability of winning future games.

I think the biggest challenge for jfeath17 is that there's only 2 years of data. I think the next step is gather data for a couple of years and see how stable that logistic regression is year-to-year. jfeath17, I'd be interested to see a more detailed procedure that you used. That way, if you ended up stepping back, someone else could try this.

Two things are going on here. One is regression toward the mean, which is a valid observation because certain statistics necessarily behave this way. The other is the idea that if something happens once the probability of it happening again is lower.This is a fallacy.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 01, 2018 03:11PM

Swampy
Two things are going on here. One is regression toward the mean, which is a valid observation because certain statistics necessarily behave this way. The other is the idea that if something happens once the probability of it happening again is lower.This is a fallacy.

Isn't the problem with regression towards the mean, knowing what the mean is? It's not the same for every team.

Just another call by the way - begging for any all of you who have ideas, to come with me and work on an enhanced model for next year. I'd be more than happy to publish it. Preferably more than one of you, so you can peer review each other :)
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.ri.ri.cox.net)
Date: March 01, 2018 10:09PM

adamw
Swampy
Two things are going on here. One is regression toward the mean, which is a valid observation because certain statistics necessarily behave this way. The other is the idea that if something happens once the probability of it happening again is lower.This is a fallacy.

Isn't the problem with regression towards the mean, knowing what the mean is? It's not the same for every team.

Just another call by the way - begging for any all of you who have ideas, to come with me and work on an enhanced model for next year. I'd be more than happy to publish it. Preferably more than one of you, so you can peer review each other :)

Well, probability and statistics has different levels of reality. The most obvious is observed empirical data. But there's also an assumption of an underlying, unobserved-but-real process that has certain probabilistic outcomes. But this applies to individual teams as well as to all teams in combination. Unless an individual team's true mean is a perfect season, which implies its probability of winning every individual game = 1.0, then since individual teams will regress towards their own means, an undefeated team will regress towards its own mean, which is < 1.0.

I don't like to explain this kind of stuff by saying things like, "assume Team X were to replay the season over 10,000 times" because it misrepresents what's actually going on with the math, and it concretizes what's actually an abstract, mathematical conceptualization. But let's do this for now.

Assume the actual probability distribution of Cornell's 1970 team going undefeated has an expected value of 0.95. In other words, if the team could replay the season an infinite number of times, 95% of the time it would go undefeated.

Since the quality of opponents varies each game, the probabilities of winning the individual games vary too. But for any given game there are two probabilities of interest. If Cornell is playing Harvard, for example, there's the probability Cornell will beat Harvard. If these given Cornell and Harvard teams were to play each other an infinite number of times, there's a certain underlying probability that Cornell would beat Harvard, but unless the distribution function of that probability has zero variance, Harvard has a non-zero probability of winning sometimes. (See my earlier post on variance.) The mean of the distribution function, its expected value, is the expected percentage of the time that Cornell would win. Suppose it really is 0.9, but after the first 10,000 games Cornell hasn't lost yet. Then, statistical theory says the tendency going forward would be for Cornell to lose because the mean really 0.9 and not 1.0, and one can prove mathematically that outcomes of probabilistic processes regress towards the mean.

The other probability is the probability of winning x games out of N games played. If x=N, then it's the probability of being undefeated at game N. The same logic applies. Knowing the "true" probabilities of winning an individual games against given opponents, we have probability distributions of winning Game 1, Game 2, etc. From these, we can construct a new variable, the probability of winning x games out of N. Again, unless this probability is 100%, then regression towards the mean implies that there's a higher likelihood an undefeated team will lose rather than win. This is because the expected value of the number of wins as of Game N, i.e. the mean, is < 1 but the number of wins up to that point = 1.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: BearLover (---.nycmny.ftas.verizon.net)
Date: March 01, 2018 11:44PM

adamw
But, as someone said, you'd need to come to the issue with actual valid math, and a better algorithm, before getting all hot and bothered about it. Until then, saying that you "feel" that 52% chance is "flawed" is just as "flawed" of an argument as anything else.
I think this discussion is getting old too, but since some people keep saying those criticizing the model are doing so based on "feel," I just want to say that we really aren't. (a) jfeath17 already showed KRACH overstates the chances of higher-ranked teams winning an individual game. (b) When combining several artificially inflated individual probabilities together (Cornell's chances of winning the quarters, semis, and finals) to form one joint probability (Cornell winning the ECAC tournament), you end up with a very, very overly inflated likelihood (the 55% chance of Cornell winning the ECAC). (c) There are no betting odds for any NHL game that come close to the odds this model is assigning many games every weekend.

KGR11
There may be confidence questions with jfeath17's model (ideally, we'd want more than 2 years of data), but it answers the question that's been raised: it takes the KRACH reported winning percentage and turns into the winning percentage that actually happened.
Right, changing the KRACH-inferred winning percentage to empirically based winning percentages would fix this problem with the model.

To make the model even more accurate would require throwing out KRACH or any ranking system that looks at only wins and losses, and instead measuring a team by goal differential, or better yet, shot differential (and adjusting for strength of schedule), but that's beyond the scope of my specific gripe with the model. (This isn't to say that ranking teams for tournament seeding/qualification purposes should look at anything other than wins/losses--KRACH is still the best at that.)

andyw2100
I can't tell if the head-smack is for me, because I'm missing something, or if it's an acknowledgment of the fact that you weren't thinking completely clearly when you made the initial post.
Me being dumb.
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 03/01/2018 11:45PM by BearLover.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 12:19AM

BearLover
Right, changing the KRACH-inferred winning percentage to empirically based winning percentages would fix this problem with the model.

Is KRACH not empirically based?

BearLover
To make the model even more accurate would require throwing out KRACH or any ranking system that looks at only wins and losses, and instead measuring a team by goal differential, or better yet, shot differential (and adjusting for strength of schedule), but that's beyond the scope of my specific gripe with the model. (This isn't to say that ranking teams for tournament seeding/qualification purposes should look at anything other than wins/losses--KRACH is still the best at that.)

It is not certain that looking at things beyond wins and losses is any better. Goal differential has major flaws, and might not mean much. Shot differential has its own issues, but could be a decent factor. Honestly, I'm not all that interested in things like goal and shot differential.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.ri.ri.cox.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 08:25AM

I wonder, is there anything like KRACH in any competitive team sport that anyone here believes does a good job predicting outcomes of individual games?
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 10:19AM

Swampy
I wonder, is there anything like KRACH in any competitive team sport that anyone here believes does a good job predicting outcomes of individual games?

SRS is used by Sports Reference and its family of sites - which I also work for. It's essentially KRACH but with score differential taken into account. I don't necessarily think that's better or worse. I've suggested they add something that gives less weight to the difference as it increases. It's being considered. Otherwise, I don't know.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: abmarks (---.hsd1.ma.comcast.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 12:21PM

it would be interesting to run a KRACH computation for last year's full NHL regular season, for example, and then see if the numbers pass people's gut checks or not.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: nshapiro (192.148.195.---)
Date: March 02, 2018 01:15PM

Swampy
Assume the actual probability distribution of Cornell's 1970 team going undefeated has an expected value of 0.95. In other words, if the team could replay the season an infinite number of times, 95% of the time it would go undefeated.
Why would you assume such a high number?

To achieve an expected value for the season of .95, you would have to assume a probability of winning each game is over .998.

If we assume the probability of Cornell '70 winning any game is .95, then the probability of an undefeated season is .226, and (don't pillory me please) that feels better.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: KGR11 (---.nycmny.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 01:21PM

BearLover
I think this discussion is getting old too, but since some people keep saying those criticizing the model are doing so based on "feel," I just want to say that we really aren't. (a) jfeath17 already showed KRACH overstates the chances of higher-ranked teams winning an individual game. (b) When combining several artificially inflated individual probabilities together (Cornell's chances of winning the quarters, semis, and finals) to form one joint probability (Cornell winning the ECAC tournament), you end up with a very, very overly inflated likelihood (the 55% chance of Cornell winning the ECAC). (c) There are no betting odds for any NHL game that come close to the odds this model is assigning many games every weekend.

Just to be clear, jfeath17's analysis shows that KRACH overstates the winning percentage for higher ranked teams if the KRACH winning % is greater than 65%. Otherwise, KRACH slightly underestimates the favorite team. Because of Cornell's impressive record, this nuance would only impact games against teams as good or better than Clarkson. It'd be interesting to see if that holds true for future seasons.

Using jfeath17's work would imply that the "true" probability of a game's outcome is based on the historical results of games with a similar KRACH difference between opponents. This assumption cannot be correct because each team is different, but how incorrect it is is dependent on the variability.

The current probability matrix says "What happens if teams maintain their current winning percentage going forward (adjusted for strength of schedule)?"
Using jfeath17's methodology, it would says "What happens if teams perform as well as the historical average team with similar KRACH ratings?"

Both are great questions with meaningful answers, but for forecasting, I would trust a matrix with jfeath17's adjustment slightly more than the current probability matrix (with the BIG assumption that the variation in results isn't ridiculous).
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Swampy (---.ri.ri.cox.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 02:29PM

nshapiro
Swampy
Assume the actual probability distribution of Cornell's 1970 team going undefeated has an expected value of 0.95. In other words, if the team could replay the season an infinite number of times, 95% of the time it would go undefeated.
Why would you assume such a high number?

To achieve an expected value for the season of .95, you would have to assume a probability of winning each game is over .998.

If we assume the probability of Cornell '70 winning any game is .95, then the probability of an undefeated season is .226, and (don't pillory me please) that feels better.
t
It was for expository, illustrative purposes only. Tha's why I said, "Assume ... ."

In general, when we talk about using any instrument for prediction, we're making very strong ceteris paribus assumptions. We're assuming or predicting that nothing that can affect the outcome changes.

Ironically, this may be legitimate for sports if we use fewer years rather than more. The more years we use, the greater the possibility of relevant differences -- rules changes, differences in conditioning, emphasis on speed vs size, injuries, etc.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Beeeej (Moderator)
Date: March 02, 2018 02:38PM

Swampy
Ironically, this may be legitimate for sports if we use fewer years rather than more. The more years we use, the greater the possibility of relevant differences -- rules changes, differences in conditioning, emphasis on speed vs size, injuries, etc.

Over time, I would expect that effect to be somewhat tempered by other multi-year factors such as recruiting parity and a more even distribution of injuries.

 
___________________________
Beeeej, Esq.

"Cornell isn't an organization. It's a loose affiliation of independent fiefdoms united by a common hockey team."
- Steve Worona
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: abmarks (---.hsd1.ma.comcast.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 03:38PM

KGR11
BearLover
I think this discussion is getting old too, but since some people keep saying those criticizing the model are doing so based on "feel," I just want to say that we really aren't. (a) jfeath17 already showed KRACH overstates the chances of higher-ranked teams winning an individual game. (b) When combining several artificially inflated individual probabilities together (Cornell's chances of winning the quarters, semis, and finals) to form one joint probability (Cornell winning the ECAC tournament), you end up with a very, very overly inflated likelihood (the 55% chance of Cornell winning the ECAC). (c) There are no betting odds for any NHL game that come close to the odds this model is assigning many games every weekend.

Just to be clear, jfeath17's analysis shows that KRACH overstates the winning percentage for higher ranked teams if the KRACH winning % is greater than 65%. Otherwise, KRACH slightly underestimates the favorite team. Because of Cornell's impressive record, this nuance would only impact games against teams as good or better than Clarkson. It'd be interesting to see if that holds true for future seasons.

Using jfeath17's work would imply that the "true" probability of a game's outcome is based on the historical results of games with a similar KRACH difference between opponents. This assumption cannot be correct because each team is different, but how incorrect it is is dependent on the variability.

The current probability matrix says "What happens if teams maintain their current winning percentage going forward (adjusted for strength of schedule)?"
Using jfeath17's methodology, it would says "What happens if teams perform as well as the historical average team with similar KRACH ratings?"

Both are great questions with meaningful answers, but for forecasting, I would trust a matrix with jfeath17's adjustment slightly more than the current probability matrix (with the BIG assumption that the variation in results isn't ridiculous).

Well said. Some people are taking jfeath's work as gospel, when in fact all we got was a simple chart showing krach win% v actual for specific, arbitrary bands. Noone double-checked the work, either.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 04:15PM

abmarks
it would be interesting to run a KRACH computation for last year's full NHL regular season, for example, and then see if the numbers pass people's gut checks or not.

This is honestly an unnecessary exercise. For past results, it's hard to improve on KRACH. The KRACH ratings, if you played the schedule that already happened, would come out to the actual results. That's the whole point of KRACH's existence.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations
Posted by: adamw (---.phlapa.fios.verizon.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 05:20PM

Just for youse guys - I was able to work up NHL KRACH - for sh**s and giggles.

           Rating   RRWP   W-L-T      Pct    Ratio     SOS
 1   TBL   189.7   .6555   40-19-6   .6615   1.955    97.0
 2   VEG   189.4   .6552   39-20-4   .6508   1.864   101.7
 3   NSH   177.5   .6401   35-18-10  .6349   1.739   102.0
 4   BOS   154.6   .6076   36-21-5   .6210   1.638    94.4
 5   WPG   147.0   .5953   35-24-4   .5873   1.423   103.3
 6   TOR   127.3   .5603   33-25-8   .5606   1.276    99.8
 7   MIN   126.9   .5595   33-26-5   .5547   1.246   101.9
 8   PIT   124.6   .5548   34-27-4   .5538   1.241   100.4
 9   DAL   122.6   .5509   32-26-6   .5469   1.207   101.6
10   LAK   119.2   .5438   34-29-2   .5385   1.167   102.2
11   COL   118.5   .5424   33-28-2   .5397   1.172   101.1
12   PHI   118.2   .5418   32-25-7   .5547   1.246    94.9
13   WSH   117.4   .5402   33-27-4   .5469   1.207    97.3
14   SJS   111.0   .5261   31-27-7   .5308   1.131    98.1
15   STL   110.7   .5254   32-30-3   .5154   1.063   104.1
16   CGY   109.1   .5218   30-28-7   .5154   1.063   102.6
17   ANA   104.9   .5121   27-26-11  .5078   1.032   101.7
18   NJD   100.8   .5020   29-27-8   .5156   1.065    94.7
19   FLA   100.6   .5016   28-28-5   .5000   1.000   100.6
20   CBJ    92.8   .4812   26-28-10  .4844    .939    98.7
21   NYI    80.5   .4458   26-33-5   .4453    .803   100.2
22   NYR    78.3   .4389   25-32-7   .4453    .803    97.5
23   CAR    77.8   .4375   25-33-6   .4375    .778   100.1
24   CHI    75.3   .4295   26-36-2   .4219    .730   103.2
25   EDM    70.6   .4135   24-36-4   .4062    .684   103.1
26   VAN    68.4   .4061   24-37-3   .3984    .662   103.3
27   MTL    65.7   .3963   22-34-7   .4048    .680    96.6
28   DET    62.1   .3828   22-36-5   .3889    .636    97.6
29   OTT    59.1   .3711   19-35-8   .3710    .590   100.2
30   ARI    52.4   .3431   18-39-6   .3333    .500   104.8
31   BUF    46.8   .3177   20-43-1   .3203    .471    99.2

The range is obviously much more narrow, which would make odds much lower even for top teams.

Compare to current NCAA range ... 533 to 15
Edited 1 time(s). Last edit at 03/02/2018 05:22PM by adamw.
 
Re: 2018 ECAC Permutations - ECAC tournament draw
Posted by: Trotsky (---.dc.dc.cox.net)
Date: March 02, 2018 05:28PM

That really quantifies how much Buffalo sucks.
 
Page:  1 2Next
Current Page: 1 of 2

Sorry, only registered users may post in this forum.

Click here to login